Bell Curve:
No bell, no curve, no...
Note: The following is the full text of a letter to the editor
published in abbreviated form in the Nation, 268(8), September 21, 1998.
To the Editor:
If I were king, I might well order my scientists to find the genes for kindness,
as Prof. Williams suggests in her column ("Invictus", The Nation,
June 15/22, 1998).
But I'm not. I'm only the editor of Psychological Science, and so I
only get to sift and winnow the best, most interesting material from what is
submitted to me.
So when a paper by M.J. Chorney, Robert Plomin, and their associates crossed my
desk, reporting the discovery of a gene implicated in IQ, the decision to
publish it was, as we say in cognitive neuroscience, a no-brainer. It is
now pretty clear to almost everyone that genetic factors count for a nontrivial
proportion of population variance in IQ -- perhaps as much as half, with the
rest accounted for by the environment. If IQ is to some extent heritable,
then there must be genes for it; and it
is of some theoretical interest to know what and where they might be. The
research in question was methodologically sophisticated, and the investigators
entered appropriate cautions about the practical, as opposed to the theoretical,
significance of their work. I was happy to publish the paper.
Contrary to Prof. Williams's implications, my confident prediction about the
rise of a new branch of the genetic testing industry wasn't based on any sense
of scientific hubris -- much less an ignorance of the subjunctive mood.
Rather, it was based on a progressive analysis of American culture -- one
largely informed, I might add, by an adult lifetime's reading of The Nation.
We're already well along the road to designer babies, after all. Some
parents, in search of the "perfect" child off the shelf, will want to
know whether their kids have this gene, and some biotechnology entrepreneurs
will be only too happy to charge them a fee for this information.
But even from a strict scientific point of view, such testing would be a crummy
idea.
By Chorney et al.'s estimate, the gene in question, IGF2R, accounts for at most
2% of the population variance in IQ. But IQ accounts for only about 10% of
the variance in important social outcomes. That means that IGF2R accounts
for only about 2/10 of 1% of the variance in educational achievement, lifetime
income, socioeconomic status, and the like. Claude Fischer and his
colleagues, in Inequality for Design (Princeton, 1996), show clearly that
the major sources of social inequality lie not in IQ, much less in IQ genes, but
in the social environment. Family funds would be better invested in higher
school budgets than on genetic testing.
Never mind the pressures placed on children who test positive for the gene, and
the adverse consequences for those who don't, when there is no reason to believe
that an IQ of 160 buys the bearer much more success, or any more happiness, than
one of 140 or 120 -- or, for that matter, 100.
If I were a behavior geneticist interested in intelligence, I would worry about
IQ as my measure of general intelligence ("g"), and indeed
about the whole concept of g. There are good reasons for thinking
that IQ tests mostly measure acquired knowledge, not raw intellectual ability,
that g is, at best, only a feeble trait, and that the real action in
intelligence may lie elsewhere, in abstract reasoning ability and in specific
cognitive abilities that are only weakly linked together. Robert
Sternberg made these points powerfully in his review of The Bell Curve (PS,
September 1995). I was also happy to publish this paper: it was one of the
few commentaries on TBC to offer technical, as opposed to ideological,
criticisms of its assumptions, methods, and conclusions.
And if I were an intelligence researcher, I would be more interested in
environmental than in genetic contributions to intelligence. And I would
be especially interested in research by Joel Myerson and his colleagues, which
re-analyzed TBC's data and found, despite Herrnstein and Murray's
denials, that there are returns to schooling after all. Specifically,
while college-educated blacks, on average, don't get much out of their high
schools compared to their white counterparts, they get much more out of college,
so that a college education cuts in half the black-white disparity in IQ scores
apparent at the end of junior high. I was also happy to publish this paper
(PS, March 1998): a better scientific justification for efforts to
improve inner-city high schools, and for affirmative action in college
admissions, would be hard to find.
It is perhaps not surprising that the right-wing press would ignore these
papers. And it is perhaps not surprising that the New York Times,
which seems obsessed with the genetic, evolutionary, neural, and hormonal
substrates of personality and social behavior, would ignore them as well.
But if I were a columnist for this country's most important progressive journal
of politics and culture, I'd certainly be interested.